Most Read... John McAuliffeBill Manhire in Conversation with John McAuliffe
(PN Review 259)
Patricia CraigVal Warner: A Reminiscence
(PN Review 259)
Eavan BolandA Lyric Voice at Bay
(PN Review 121)
Joshua WeinerAn Exchange with Daniel Tiffany/Fall 2020
(PN Review 259)
Vahni CapildeoOn Judging Prizes, & Reading More than Six Really Good Books
(PN Review 237)
Christopher MiddletonNotes on a Viking Prow
(PN Review 10)
Next Issue Kirsty Gunn re-arranges the world John McAuliffe reads Seamus Heaney's letters and translations Chris Price's 'Songs of Allegiance' David Herman on Aharon Appelfeld Victoria Moul on Christopher Childers compendious Greek and Latin Lyric Book Philip Terry again answers the question, 'What is Poetry'
Poems Articles Interviews Reports Reviews Contributors
Reader Survey
PN Review Substack

This article is taken from PN Review 123, Volume 25 Number 1, September - October 1998.

Against Transcendental Gossip: the symbolic language of rhythm Chris McCully

In this essay I'd like to follow up one of the ideas outlined in my reply to Peter Goldsworthy's stimulating article in PNR 114. What is rhythm for? Why are human beings apparently endowed with a potential for apprehending, and re-creating, highly organised rhythmic structures? What, in evolutionary and aesthetic terms, are these structures doing within human cognitive processes? One might agree with Stevens that 'It must be abstract... It must change... It must give pleasure...' Yes - but why?

Children know about, and enjoy, rhythm. Humans seem to be innately, and possible uniquely, endowed among the higher primates with the capacity to inhabit, and to recreate, three intersecting forms of aural structure. These forms are found in (i) sequence, (ii) contiguity, and (iii) hierarchy.

In a sequence, something recurs. Rhythmically, the something that recurs is a point of salience (in linguistic terms, of markedness). Languages differ in how they register salience. In some languages - Classical Greek seems to have been one - vowel length functions as salient, so that a long vowel or diphthong is perceived to be more prominent than a short vowel. In other languages, stressed syllables (not just vowels within syllables) function as salient, while unstressed syllables are non-salient. Modern English is an example. Here, contrasts of salience are optimized: stressed syllables occur predictably, and their vowels are uttered with full quality; unstressed syllables can be recognised as non-salient by their 'reduced' quality, their typical production with the 'murmur vowel', ...


Searching, please wait... animated waiting image